Study shows how auditory perception and motor processes work together

Reviewed by Emily Henderson, B.Sc.

How do people coordinate their actions with the sounds they hear? This basic ability, which allows people to cross the street safely while hearing oncoming traffic, dance to new music or perform team events such as rowing, has puzzled cognitive neuroscientists for years.

A new study led by researchers at McGill University is shining a light on how auditory perception and motor processes work together.

Keeping the beat – it takes more than just moving or listening well

In a recent paper in the Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, the researchers, led by Caroline Palmer, a professor in McGill’s Department of Psychology, were able to identify neural markers of musicians’ beat perceptions.

Surprisingly, these markers did not correspond to the musician’s ability to either hear or produce a beat – only to their ability to synchronize with it.

The authors, as performing musicians, are familiar with musical situations in which one performer is not correctly aligned in time with fellow performers – so we were interested in exploring how musician’s brains respond to rhythms. It could be that some people are better musicians because they listen differently or it could be that they move their bodies differently.

We found that the answer was a match between the pulsing or oscillations in the brain rhythms and the pulsing of the musical rhythm – it’s not just listening or movement. It’s a linking of the brain rhythm to the auditory rhythm.”

Caroline Palmer, Study Senior Author and Professor, Department of Psychology, McGill University

Related Stories
Brain protein associated with seizures and abnormal social behaviors
Circadian rhythms help in brain’s unique process of waste removal
Study examines the brain’s reactions in conditions of uncertainty
Palmer is also the Canada Research Chair in Cognitive Neuroscience of Performance.

Super-synchronizers – an exception or a learnable skill?

The researchers used electroencephalography (EEGs involve placing electrodes on the scalp to detect electrical activity in the brain) to measure brain activity as participants in the experiment, all of them experienced musicians, synchronized their tapping with a range of musical rhythms they were hearing.

By doing so they were able to identify neural markers of musicians’ beat perceptions that corresponded to their ability to synchronize well.

“We were surprised that even highly trained musicians sometimes showed reduced ability to synchronize with complex rhythms, and that this was reflected in their EEGs,” said co-first authors Brian Mathias and Anna Zamm, both PhD students in the Palmer lab. “Most musicians are good synchronizers; nonetheless, this signal was sensitive enough to distinguish the “good” from the “better” or “super-synchronizers”, as we sometimes call them.”

It’s not clear whether anyone can become a super-synchronizer, but according to Palmer, the lead researcher, it may be possible to improve ones ability to synchronize.

“The range of musicians we sampled suggests that the answer would be yes. And the fact that only 2-3 % of the population are ‘beat deaf’ is also encouraging. Practice definitely improves your ability and improves the alignment of the brain rhythms with the musical rhythms. But whether everyone is going to be as good as a drummer is not clear.”

Source:
McGill University

Journal reference:
Mathias, B., et al. (2020) Rhythm Complexity Modulates Behavioral and Neural Dynamics During Auditory–Motor Synchronization. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01601.

Currently rated 5.0 by 1 person

Posted in: Medical Science News | Medical Research News

Tags: Brain, Language, Neuroscience, Psychology, Research, Tongue

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published.